Running Head: PARADOX OF PARSIMONY Comment: The Paradox of Parsimony in Motivated Distance Perception
نویسنده
چکیده
The motivated distance perception theory (Balcetis, 2015) paradoxically is too parsimonious to account for a variety of findings, including those of the author. The theory poorly defines the features of eliciting situations, which fails to constrain the theory making it non-falsifiable and allows for post-hoc interpretation of the effects. Finally, the theory ignores the complexity of the motivational system and the automaticity of motivations. Paradox of Parsimony 3 Comment: The Paradox of Parsimony in Motivated Distance Perception The motivated distance perception theory is quite parsimonious (approach = closer; avoidance = farther); however, there are several concerns that limit its utility. First, there is a lack of clarity in defining the eliciting features that shift perceivers’ motivational direction. Second, the theory fails to recognize the complexity of the motivational systems and its core principles. Third, the theory ignores the importance of assessing both relative and absolute measures of distance to stimuli. Overall, the theory has limited predictability, it is not falsifiable, and more importantly it fails to account for and explain perceptual biases. Parsimony and Predictability Why is it important to provide a more comprehensive theory that clearly identifies the features of eliciting situations? Because it can lead to post-hoc interpretation as exhibited by the author. For instance, Cole, Balcetis, and Dunning (2013) argued that spiders evoke avoidance tendencies, but in the present review when discussing the same data the author suggests that spiders evoke approach-oriented tendencies. Thus, the data, rather than the theory, seem to determine the motivational direction. The theory, unfortunately, allows for this convenient switch in motivation by having the ‘features of eliciting situations’ precede the motivation stage, which implies that the situation governs motivation. This implication, however, ignores a core principle of motivation: emotions, such as fear and disgust, automatically evoke a motivation tendency (Cisler et al., 2009). A person who sees a spider will automatically activate an avoidance tendency, thus how does avoidance become approach? To resolve this paradox, the model should incorporate an appraisal mechanism that either runs parallel to or after the automatic activation of the motivational tendency. Otherwise, eliciting features of situations will drive motivation, which contradicts the authors own theoretical argument. Paradox of Parsimony 4 Limiting motivation to the global behaviors of approach and avoid is too simplistic and fails to capture the complexity of the motivational system. Given the data in the review and using a simplistic motivational model, the theory cannot account for why disgust and fear, two avoidant motivations, produced proximal and distal biases and accurate perception. But, the theory could account for the data better if perception was influenced by sub-behaviors of avoidance (fight, flight, freezing; Corr & McNaughton, 2012), which can increase the flexibility of the model, but maintain predictability. Avoidance behaviors that evoke fight, flight, or freezing could bias perception proximally, distally, and accurately, respectively. Moreover, the theory lacks a clear prediction for conflict between the two motivational systems even though motivational theories suggest that conflict produces cautious approach. Unfortunately, the author uses conflict to justify non-predicted results (i.e., disgust). Eliciting Features Greater clarity is required when defining features of eliciting situations and how those features bias motivation and perception. For instance, the author presents contradictory results for how the availability of resources influence perception. Specifically, threat without the ability to escape leads to proximal estimates when resources are depleted (Cole et al., 2013; Harber et al., 2011) and when resources are ample (Cesario et al., 2014). Elsewhere in the review, individuals threated and who have available resources evoke both accurate (Harber et al., 2011) and proximal (Cesario et al., 2010) estimates. Thus, a lack of clarity allows for all situational factors to be freely interpreted creating a non-falsifiable theory. Measurement Issues The motivated perception theory fails to provide recommendations for how perceptual estimates should be measured, which impacts the reliability and validity of the results. When Paradox of Parsimony 5 identifying motivated perception effects, there should be at least two analyses conducted. First, the “motivated” stimulus must be compared to a control stimulus. Second, the “motivated” stimulus must be compared to the motivated stimulus’ actual distance. In much of the work reported, the motivated stimulus was never compared to the actual distance. Interestingly, motivated stimuli that appeared proximal to a control stimulus were sometimes perceived to be proximal to itself and sometimes perceived accurately to itself (Balcetis & Dunning, 2010; Cole & Balcetis, 2013). Should we assume that these effects shared the same underlying mechanism? Was a confounding variable present in one situation, but not the other? Failure to provide clarity for how to reconcile these inconsistencies reduce the validity and reliability of the effects and the underlying theory. Concluding Remarks The motivated distance perception theory provides less clarity and understanding for why motivation influences perception than other functional accounts of perception (e.g., Proffitt, 2006). Paradox of Parsimony 6 ReferencesBalcetis, E., & Dunning, D. (2010). Wishful seeing: More desired objects are seen as closer.Psychological Science, 21, 147-152.Cesario, J., & Navarrete, C. D. (2014). Perceptual bias in threat distance: The critical roles ofingroup support and target evaluations in defensive threat regulation. SocialPsychological and Personality Science, 5, 12-17. Cesario, J., Plaks, J. E., Hagiwara, N., Navarrete, C. D., & Higgins, E. T. (2010). The ecology ofautomaticity: How situational contingencies shape action semantics and social behavior.Psychological Science, 21(9), 1311-1317.Cisler, J., Olatunji, B., &Lohr;, J. (2009). Disgust, fear, and the anxiety disorders: A criticalreview. Clinical psychology review, 29, 34-46.Cole, S., & Balcetis, E. (2013). Sources of resources: Bioenergetic and psychoenergeticresources influence distance perception. Social Cognition, 31(6), 721-732.Cole, S., Balcetis, E., & Dunning, D. (2013). Affective signals of threat increase perceivedproximity. Psychological Science, 24, 34-40.Corr, P., & McNaughton, N. (2012). Neuroscience and approach/avoidance personality traits: Atwo stage (valuation-motivation) approach. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 36,2339-2354.Harber, K. D., Yeung, D., &Iacovelli;, A. (2011). Psychosocial resources, threat, and theperception of distance and height: Support for the resources and perception model.Emotion, 11(5), 1080-1090.Proffitt, D.R. (2006). Embodied perception and the economy of action. Perspectives onPsychological Science, 1, 110–122.
منابع مشابه
The Paradox of Health Policy: Revealing the True Colours of This ‘Chameleon Concept’; Comment on “The Politics and Analytics of Health Policy”
Health policy has been termed a ‘chameleon concept’, referring to its ability to take on different forms of disciplinarity as well as different roles and functions. This paper extends Paton’s analysis by exploring the paradox of health policy as a field of academic inquiry—sitting across many of the boundaries of social science but also marginalised by them. It situates contemporary approaches ...
متن کاملThe Paradox of Intervening in Complex Adaptive Systems; Comment on “Using Complexity and Network Concepts to Inform Healthcare Knowledge Translation”
This commentary addresses two points raised by Kitson and colleagues’ article. First, increasing interest in applying the Complexity Theory lens in healthcare needs further systematic work to create some commonality between concepts used. Second, our need to adopt a better understanding of how these systems organise so we can change the systems overall behaviour, creates a paradox. We seek to m...
متن کاملAdiposopathy, “Sick Fat,” Ockham’s Razor, and Resolution of the Obesity Paradox
Among lean populations, cardiovascular disease (CVD) is rare. Among those with increased adiposity, CVD is the commonest cause of worldwide death. The "obesity paradox" describes seemingly contrary relationships between body fat and health/ill-health. Multiple obesity paradoxes exist, and include the anatomic obesity paradox, physiologic obesity paradox, demographic obesity paradox, therapeutic...
متن کاملParadox in the Poetry of Hazin-e Lahiji
Paradox is one of the literary techniques in the poetry of the Safavid poets. Hazin-e Lahiji, like so many other poets of that age, employed this technique in his pursuit and showed that "unfamiliar meaning". Paradox is used in the poetry of Hazin-e Lahiji for the purpose of defamiliarization and exoticism. The poet in order to create new implications and subtle and insightful points and also t...
متن کاملذخیره در منابع من
با ذخیره ی این منبع در منابع من، دسترسی به آن را برای استفاده های بعدی آسان تر کنید
عنوان ژورنال:
دوره شماره
صفحات -
تاریخ انتشار 2015